Skip to main content
  • Practices and Applications
  • Open access
  • Published:

Negligible Influence of Spatial Autocorrelation in the Assessment of Fire Effects in a Mixed Conifer Forest

Abstract

Fire is an important feature of many forest ecosystems, although the quantification of its effects is compromised by the large scale at which fire occurs and its inherent unpredictability. A recurring problem is the use of subsamples collected within individual burns, potentially resulting in spatially autocorrelated data. Using subsamples from six different fires (and three unburned control areas) we show little evidence for strong spatial autocorrelation either before or after burning for eight measures of forest conditions (both fuels and vegetation). Additionally, including a term for spatially autocorrelated errors provided little improvement for simple linear models contrasting the effects of early versus late season burning. While the effects of spatial autocorrelation should always be examined, it may not always greatly influence assessments of fire effects. If high patch scale variability is common in Sierra Nevada mixed conifer forests, even following more than a century of fire exclusion, treatments designed to encourage further heterogeneity in forest conditions prior to the reintroduction of fire will likely be unnecessary.

Introduction

Fire is fundamental in shaping most terrestrial ecosystems (Bond and Keeley 2005). However, understanding fire effects remains elusive in part because fires are typically unplanned, not normally under experimental control, and occur at spatial scales of hundreds to thousands of hectares; these features are at odds with classical experimental design and analysis. Experimental burning in forests, where it has been attempted, is constrained by the logistical and financial resources needed to conduct the fires, limiting the size, intensity and number of replicated burning treatments (Fulé et al. 2004, Stephens and Moghaddas 2005, Fulé et al. 2006, North et al. 2007, Schwilk et al. 2009). Opportunistic studies of fire effects frequently rely on data collected from subsamples within a single burned area. These subsamples may be correlated both spatially and temporally, and when subjected to standard statistical testing provide reduced estimates of variation (error), increasing the likelihood of committing a Type I error (the chance of detecting a significant effect of fire when no meaningful effect has occurred). That is, the subsampled data underlying these tests are pseudoreplicated (Hurlbert 1984).

Interpreting pseudoreplicated fire effects data will always present challenges (van Mantgem et al. 2001), but some of these difficulties could be mitigated by estimating spatial autocorrelation (the correspondence of nearby sampling units) and temporal autocorrelation (the similarity of samples measured repeatedly over time), and controlling for these relationships in analyses of fire effects (Legendre 1993, Legendre and Legendre 1998, Fortin and Dale 2005, Bataineh et al. 2006). It is unclear; however, to what degree the consideration of autocorrelation, particularly spatial autocorrelation, would improve our understanding of fire effects. Small scale heterogeneity in fire effects may be common as daily and seasonal differences in fire weather and fuel moisture interact with variability in topography, fuel loading, and vegetation during burning (Kilgore 1973, Knapp and Keeley 2006).

The degree of spatial heterogeneity also has implications for an ongoing debate concerning the need for mechanical thinning prior to the reintroduction of prescribed fire in Sierran mixed conifer forests. Arguments in favor of pre-fire thinning are based on the notion that a century of fire exclusion has led to the homogenization of previously heterogeneous stands, and the application of fire without preceding silvicultural treatments will perpetuate these changes in forest structure (Bonnicksen and Stone 1981, 1982; Bonnicksen 1989). Here we show that there is only weak evidence for pervasive spatial autocorrelation both before and after prescription burning for measures of fire effects relevant to managers, and that spatial autocorrelation had trivial effects when comparing the outcomes of early versus late season burning in a Sierra Nevada mixed conifer forest.

Methods

Study Site

We conducted the study in an old growth mixed conifer forest within the Giant Forest region of Sequoia National Park, California, USA. The sites have never been logged. Frequent fires characterized the forests prior to Euro-American settlement, but the area containing the study plots has not burned since the late 1800s (Swetnam et al. 1992). The climate is Mediterranean, with hot, dry summers and cool, wet winters, with about half of annual precipitation falling as snow (Stephenson 1988). Soils are relatively young (mostly inceptisols) and derived from granitic parent material.

Burning Treatments

We compared the effects of early season burning, late season burning and no burning across nine experimental units using data from the southern Sierra Nevada node of the Fire and Fire Surrogate network (Schwilk et al. 2009) (Figure 1). The experimental units were each 15 ha to 20 ha in size and were located within larger burn areas on west to northwest facing aspects of variable slope at elevations ranging from 1900 m to 2150 m. Burning treatments were applied using a completely randomized design with three replicates per treatment.

Figure 1
figure 1

Location of the Giant Forest area within Sequoia National Park, California. Magnified area shows the plot layout for the Sequoia National Park site of the National Fire and Fire Surrogate study. Plot labels designate treatment (early season burning, late season burning, or unburned control), while rectangles within the plots represent randomly located 0.1 ha subplots where fire effects data were collected.

Early season burns were conducted on 20 and 27 June 2002. Late season burns were conducted on 28 September, and on 17 and 28 October 2001. Burns were ignited with strip head fires started at the highest point in the unit, and designed to burn at low to moderate intensities. The burns were surface fires, with a few cases of individual trees torching. Weather and fuel conditions at the time of the burns are provided in Knapp et al. (2005).

Sampling

We took all pre- and post-treatment data in plots referenced to a 50 m grid in the interior of each unit. To minimize edge effects, the grid system was surrounded by a 50 m to 100 m buffer that was also treated. We averaged fuels data (total fuels [Mg ha−1] and large fuels [woody fuels >76 mm diameter, Mg ha−1]) from two transects at each of 36 grid points per experimental unit. Forest structure data (stand density [trees >1.37 m in height ha−1], basal area [m2 ha−1]) were taken from ten 0.1 ha subplots at each experimental unit. We took community composition data (forb cover, graminoid cover, shrub cover) at the same ten 0.1 ha subplots at each experimental unit, while understory species richness data were averaged from nine 1 m2 quadrats at each 0.1 ha subplot. We measured fuels before and one year following burning. We measured trees, shrubs, and herbaceous vegetation before and three years following burning.

Statistical Tests

We used the Mantel test to measure spatial dependence among samples (Legendre 1993, Legendre and Legendre 1998, Fortin and Dale 2005). The Mantel test compares two or more distance matrices, one matrix (A ij ) being differences in the variable of interest (e.g., fuel loading, stand density, etc.), with the other matrix (B i j ) being the distance between the sampling units. The Mantel test computes the correlation between the two distance matrices, with the formula:

$$z = \sum\limits_{i = 1}^n {\sum\limits_{j = 1}^n {{A_j}\;{B_j}\;,\;{\rm{for}}\;{\rm{i}}}} \neq {\rm{j}}$$

The z statistic is usually normalized (r):

$$r = {1 \over {(n - 1)}}\sum\limits_{i = 1}^n {\sum\limits_{j = 1}^n {{{({A_j} - \overline A )} \over {{s_A}}}{{({B_j} - \overline B )} \over {{s_B}}}}}$$

where n is the number of elements in the distance matrix and s A and s B are standard deviations of the elements of the A ij and B ij matrices. The normalized statistic behaves similar to the Pearson correlation coefficient, varying between −1 and +1, so that coefficients can be compared to other variables at the same site or to similar variables at other sites. We determined the overall significance of spatial relationships by permutation testing (standard tests are unreliable because the distances in the matrices are not independent [Goslee and Urban 2007]).

We assessed spatial correlations among subplots within each experimental unit before and after burning for measures of fire effects that are relevant for resource managers, including total surface fuels, large fuels, stand density, stand basal area, herbaceous vegetation cover, graminoid vegetation cover, shrub cover, and understory species richness. We subjected each of these measures within each experimental unit to a Mantel test using Euclidean distances, with 10 000 permutations used to establish significance (α = 0.05). Although the large number of tests we performed would argue for an adjustment of the critical value, we wanted these tests to be as liberal as possible to search for evidence of significant spatial autocorrelation (e.g., if we used a Bonferroni correction for our 144 tests, we would have a critical value of α = 0.05 ÷ 144 = 0.000 35, (a value not surpassed by any of our tests). For both the pre-fire and post-fire intervals we calculated the ratio of significant Mantel tests versus the total number of tests conducted, and created 95 % confidence intervals for this ratio from 10 000 bootstrapped samples.

To determine the effects of spatial autocorrelation on an assessment of fire effects, we contrasted the results of tests that compared early versus late season burning using ordinary least-squares regression (OLS) and a spatial generalized least squares regression (GLS). The OLS model assumes no spatial autocorrelation among samples, potentially leading to excessive reductions in standard errors of the parameter estimates (and thereby inflating the probability of Type I errors), while the GLS model included spatial structure into the error term of the regression (Pinheiro and Bates 2000). For the GLS model, we used a spherical spatial error structure, with the inclusion of a nugget effect where needed (Cressie 1993). Our response variables were the change in a forest attribute (e.g., paired differences of stem densitypre-fire − stem densitypost-fire) as predicted by season of burning. Season of burning contrasts are presented relative to early season burning. Parameters were estimated using maximum likelihood and model comparisons were performed using AICc, the Akaike information criterion corrected for sample size (Burnham and Anderson 2002). Model selection is typically done by referencing the model with the lowest AICc value. Here, to emphasize the inclusion of spatially autocorrelated errors, we made our selection relative to the GLS model (Δ AICc GLS = AICc OLS AICc GLS ), with evidence to include spatial autocorrelation when Δ AICc GLS > 2, and evidence not to include spatial autocorrelation when Δ AICc GLS < −2. Strong evidence to include or exclude spatial autocorrelations is present when the absolute value of Δ AICc GLS > 10. Mantel tests were conducted using the “ecodist” package, and regression tests used the “nlme” package written for the R language (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria).

Results

The Mantel tests did not show evidence for widespread spatial autocorrelation either before or after burning for fuels (Table 1a) or vegetation (Table 1b). The pre-fire interval had nine significant Mantel tests out of 72 tests (ratio of significant tests = 0.13; 95 % CI = 0.06 to 0.22), which is only marginally greater than the frequency of significant tests expected by chance alone (i.e., 1 out of 20, or 0.05). The post-fire interval had even fewer significant Mantel tests, with the 95 % CI overlapping the number of significant tests expected by chance (seven out of 72 significant tests, ratio of significant tests = 0.10; 95 % CI = 0.03 to 0.17). There was no obvious pattern of significant results in any measure of fire effects either before or after burning, with each measure of fire effects averaging only a single significant Mantel test during both the pre- and post-fire interval (average ratio of significant tests = 0.11; range = 0.00 to 0.33).

Table 1a Normalized Mantel test statistics for pre-fire and post-fire measurements of fuels. Mantel test statistics in bold signify significant spatial correlations within experimental units at a = 0.05.
Table 1b Normalized Mantel test statistics for pre-fire and post-fire measurements of forest structure and understory community composition. Mantel test statistics in bold signify significant spatial correlations within experimental units at α = 0.05.

The lack of evidence for significant spatial autocorrelation was also reflected in our comparisons of the OLS and spatial GLS models (Table 2). In only three of eight tests (changes in stand density, forb cover, and species richness) was the inclusion of spatial autocorrelation justified by AICc. In four of eight tests, the inclusion of spatial autocorrelation was not justified (Δ AICc GLS < −2), with one test (change in basal area) giving essentially equal evidence for the OLS and GLS models. In only one test (change in forb cover) was the absolute difference in AICc > 10, suggesting that for most measures of fire effects there was approximately equivalent evidence for the OLS and GLS models. In only a single instance could the inclusion of spatial autocorrelation conceivably give rise to a different interpretation of the results when comparing P values (critical value a = 0.05, change in forb cover: OLS P = 0.058, GLS P = 0.184).

Table 2 Coefficients for changes following fire as determined by season of burning using ordinary least-squares regression (OLS) and spatial generalized least squares regression (GLS). Regression parameters (β) describe the difference of late season burning relative to early season burning. AICc values of OLS and GLS models are compared relative to the GLS model (ΔAICc GLS ).

The parameter estimates for the OLS and GLS models were generally similar. As expected, the standard errors of these estimates were consistently smaller in the OLS models relative to the GLS models (paired permutation test, P = 0.016), although the magnitude of these differences were slight (average standard error reduction in the OLS models = 13.6 %), suggesting that the OLS estimates did not greatly inflate the risks of Type I errors. Note that some of these tests imply differences in the effects of early versus late season prescribed burning, with late season burning resulting in the greater consumption of total fuels (in agreement with Knapp et al. [2005]), lesser reductions in graminoid cover and greater reductions in shrub cover.

Discussion

It appears, at least in the mixed conifer forests of the Sierra Nevada, that there is little evidence for pervasive significant spatial autocorrelation for many of the measures of fire effects important to managers (e.g., fuels, forest structure, understory community structure). We do not, however, take this as evidence that spatial autocorrelation should be ignored in statistical tests of fire effects. The ratio of significant Mantel tests was above 5 % both in the pre-fire and post-fire intervals, and some tests were highly significant. In addition, comparing linear models with and without spatial autocorrelation (OLS versus GLS models), there were a minority of tests that supported the inclusion of spatial autocorrelation (though the effects of spatial autocorrelation was minor and did not influence model interpretation). The infrequent presence of significant spatial autocorrelation precludes the creation of simple rules concerning the presence or absence of spatial autocorrelation for fire effects studies. These findings underscore the idea that the effects of spatial autocorrelation should be routinely checked in ecological studies (Legendre 1993, Legendre and Legendre 1998), although it may not often prove to be pivotal in the assessment of fire effects. As a cautionary note, Diniz-Filho et al. (2003) found that including a term for spatial autocorrelation when its effects are weak may lead to subtle biases in model interpretation (i.e., a reduced emphasis on predictors that operate at small spatial scales). If subsampled data are used as independent replicates in fire effect studies, other problems arising from pseudoreplication still remain (e.g., temporal autocorrelation, non-representative samples), though spatial autocorrelation may not heavily influence the results.

Our results imply that forest conditions before and after burning have a high degree of small, patch scale spatial heterogeneity. Knapp and Keeley (2006) also found evidence for high patch scale heterogeneity in fire severity, as measured by scorch heights and area burned, which they attributed to variation in topography, fuel characteristics and forest structure (including pre-burn species composition), and sometimes, as in our study, season of burning. Higher fuel moistures in early season burns likely inhibit effective fuel continuity, thereby increasing small scale differences in fire severity. High patch scale heterogeneity is similar to the high levels of heterogeneity in fire severity commonly observed at the landscape scale in the Sierra Nevada (Collins et al. 2007) and in other forest systems (e.g., Turner et al. 1994). We suspect that small scale heterogeneity is a general feature of fire effects, particularly in forest types that burn at low to moderate intensity (in contrast to crown fire systems that may have lower patch scale heterogeneity [Turner and Romme 1994]).

The presence of high patch scale variability in pre- and post-fire stand conditions has important consequences for the management of Sierra Nevada forests. Bonnicksen and Stone (1981, 1982) and Bonnicksen (1989) have argued that fire exclusion has resulted in the homogenization of historically heterogeneous forests, and application of prescribed fire without prior silvicultural treatment would maintain unnaturally uniform forest conditions. This view has been challenged on several accounts (e.g., imprecise knowledge of historic forest conditions [Stephenson 1999]), and our results also demonstrate that even following a century of fire exclusion, forest conditions are far from homogeneous either before or after prescription fire. Thus, the application of treatments prior to prescribed burning to encourage further stand heterogeneity either in fuels or vegetation is likely unnecessary in Sierra Nevada mixed conifer forests.

The lack of strong spatial autocorrelation also has implications for the interpretation and analysis of fire effects monitoring data. If spatial autocorrelation is generally weak, it is doubtful that one or even several small monitoring plots within a burned area will provide a general description of overall effects of a given fire. Currently, a national plot-based fire effects monitoring program (FFI, http://frames.nbii.gov/ffi) measures fuels and forest structure within small-scale plots (≤0.1 ha), usually with only a single plot established within each burned area (Paul Reeberg, National Park Service, personal communication). Besides its potential to support satellite-based observations of fire effects (Key 2006), these data might be best used when individual plot data are assembled together across a particular vegetation type to offer a broad picture of fire effects (e.g., Keifer 1998).

We conclude that the conditions and response of forests to fire are complex, and are certainly more variable than is sometimes supposed. It is an overgeneralization to consider an area as simply burned or unburned, as conditions prior to, during and following fires combine to create heterogeneous conditions. We do not, however, possess a mechanistic understanding of what drives this complexity. Some factors are obvious, such as variation in slope or vegetation types within a burned area. Other factors are surprising (e.g., pre-burn species composition [Knapp and Keeley 2006]), or will probably remain elusive (e.g., small changes in fire weather during burning), and it is likely that many of these factors interact in unexpected ways. Understanding these mechanisms is becoming increasingly important in an era where forest and fire conditions are rapidly changing (Westerling et al. 2006, van Mantgem et al. 2009), obviating simple predictions of fire effects.

Literature Cited

  • Bataineh, A.L., B.P. Oswald, M. Bataineh, D. Unger, I. Hung, and D. Scognamillo. 2006. Spatial autocorrrelation and pseudoreplication in fire ecology. Fire Ecology 2: 107–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bond, W.J., and J.E. Keeley. 2005. Fire as a global ‘herbivore’: the ecology and evolution of flammable ecosystems. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 20: 387–394.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bonnicksen, T.M. 1989. Nature vs. man(agement). Journal of Forestry 87: 41–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bonnicksen, T.M., and E.C. Stone. 1981. The giant sequoia-mixed conifer forest community characterized through pattern analysis as a mosaic of aggregations. Forest Ecology and Management 3: 307–328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bonnicksen, T.M., and E.C. Stone. 1982. Managing vegetation within US national parks: a policy analysis. Environmental Management 6: 101–102, 190–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burnham, K.P., and D.R. Anderson. 2002. Model selection and multimodel inference. Second edition. Springer-Verlag, New York, New York, USA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins, B.M., M. Kelly, J.W. van Wagtendonk, and S.L. Stephens. 2007. Spatial patterns of large natural fires in Sierra Nevada wilderness areas. Landscape Ecology 22: 545–557.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cressie, N.A.C. 1993. Statistics for spatial data. John Wiley and Sons, New York, New York, USA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diniz-Filho, J.A.F., L.M. Bini, and B.A. Hawkins. 2003. Spatial autocorrelation and red herrings in geographical ecology. Global Ecology Biogeography 12: 53–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fortin, M.J. and M.R.T. Dale. 2005. Spatial analysis: a guide for ecologists. Cambridge University Press, New York, New York, USA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fulé, P.Z., A.E. Cocke, T.A. Heinlein, and W.W. Covington. 2004. Effects of an intense prescribed forest fire: is it ecological restoration? Restoration Ecology 12: 220–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fulé, P.Z., W.W. Covington, M.T. Stoddard, and D. Bertolette. 2006. Minimal-impact restoration treatments have limited effects on forest structure and fuels at Grand Canyon, USA. Restoration Ecology 14: 357–368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goslee, S.C., and D.L. Urban. 2007. The ecodist package for dissimilarity-based analysis of ecological data. Journal of Statistical Software 22: 1–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hurlbert, S.H. 1984. Pseudoreplication and the design of ecological field experiments. Ecological Monographs 54: 187–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keifer, M. 1998. Fuel load and tree density changes following prescribed fire in the giant sequoia-mixed conifer forest: the first 14 years of fire effects monitoring. Proceedings of the Tall Timbers Fire Ecology Conference 20: 306–309.

    Google Scholar 

  • Key, C.H. 2006. Ecological and sampling constraints on defining landscape fire. Fire Ecology 2(2): 34–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kilgore, B.M. 1973. Impact of prescribed burning on a sequoia-mixed conifer forest. Proceedings of the Tall Timbers Fire Ecology Conference 12: 345–375.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knapp, E.E., and J.E. Keeley. 2006. Heterogeneity in fire severity within early season and late season prescribed burns in a mixed-conifer forest. International Journal of Wildland Fire 15: 37–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knapp, E.E., J.E. Keeley, E.A. Ballenger, and T.J. Brennan. 2005. Fuel reduction and coarse woody debris dynamics with early season and late season prescribed fire in a Sierra Nevada mixed conifer forest. Forest Ecology and Management 208: 383–397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Legendre, P. 1993. Spatial autocorrelation: trouble or new paradigm? Ecology 74: 1659–1673.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Legendre, P., and L. Legendre. 1998. Numerical ecology. Elsevier, Amersterdam, Netherlands.

    Google Scholar 

  • North, M., J. Innes, and H. Zald. 2007. Comparison of thinning and prescribed fire restoration treatments to Sierran mixed-conifer historic conditions. Canadian Journal of Forest Research 37: 331–342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinheiro, J.C., and D.M. Bates. 2000. Mixed-effects models in S and S-PLUS. Springer, New York, New York, USA.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Schwilk, D.W., J.E. Keeley, E.E. Knapp, J. McIver, J.D. Bailey, C.J. Fettig, C.E. Fiedler, R.J. Harrod, J.J. Moghaddas, K.W. Outcalt, C.N. Skinner, S.L. Stephens, T.A. Waldrop, D.A. Yaussy, and A. Youngblood. 2009. The national fire and fire surrogate study: effects of fuel reduction methods on forest vegetation structure and fuels. Ecological Applications 19: 285–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stephens, S.L., and J.J. Moghaddas. 2005. Experimental fuel treatment impacts on forest structure, potential fire behavior, and predicted tree mortality in a California mixed conifer forest. Forest Ecology and Management 215: 21–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stephenson, N.L. 1988. Climatic control of vegetation distribution: the role of the water-balance with examples from North America and Sequoia National Park, California. Dissertation, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York, USA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stephenson, N.L. 1999. Reference conditions for giant sequoia forest restoration: structure, process, and precision. Ecological Applications 9: 1253–1265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swetnam, T.W., C.H. Baisan, A.C. Caprio, R. Touchan, and P.M. Brown. 1992. Tree-ring reconstruction of giant sequoia fire regimes. University of Arizona Laboratory of Tree-Ring Research Report. Tucson, Arizona, USA.

  • Turner, M.G., W.W. Hargrove, R.H. Gardner, and W.H. Romme. 1994. Effects of fire on landscape heterogeneity in Yellowstone National Park, Wyoming. Journal of Vegetation Science 5: 731–742.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turner, M.G., and W.H. Romme. 1994. Landscape dynamics in crown fire ecosystems. Landscape Ecology 9: 59–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Mantgem, P., M. Schwartz, and M. Keifer. 2001. Monitoring fire effects for managed burns and wildfires: coming to terms with pseudoreplication. Natural Areas Journal 21: 266–273.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Mantgem, P.J., N.L. Stephenson, J.C. Byrne, L.D. Daniels, J.F. Franklin, P.Z. Fulé, M.E. Harmon, A.J. Larson, J.M. Smith, A.H. Taylor, and T.T. Veblen. 2009. Widespread increase of tree mortality rates in the western United States. Science 323: 521–524.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Westerling, A.L., H.G. Hidalgo, D.R. Cayan, and T.W. Swetnam. 2006. Warming and earlier spring increase western US forest wildfire activity. Science 313: 940–943.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank J. Keeley and E. Knapp for their management of the Sequoia National Park Fire and Fire Surrogate site and the field crewmembers who collected much of the data; A. Das, N. Stephenson, and two anonymous reviewers for helpful comments on the manuscript; and J. Yee for statistical advice. This paper is a contribution from the Western Mountain Initiative (a US Geological Survey global change research project), the Cordillera Forest Dynamics Network (CORFOR), and contribution #196 of the National Fire and Fire Surrogate Study, funded by the USDA/DOI Joint Fire Science Program. Any use of trade names is for descriptive purposes only and does not imply endorsement by the US government.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Phillip J. van Mantgem.

Rights and permissions

Open Access  This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made.

The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder.

To view a copy of this licence, visit https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

van Mantgem, P.J., Schwilk, D.W. Negligible Influence of Spatial Autocorrelation in the Assessment of Fire Effects in a Mixed Conifer Forest. fire ecol 5, 116–125 (2009). https://doi.org/10.4996/fireecology.0502116

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.4996/fireecology.0502116

Keywords